The heart is an efficient machine that supplies the body with oxygenated blood and recycles unoxygenated blood (Weedman 2009). Unoxygenated blood comes into the heart, is pumped to the lungs, oxygenated, and then returned to the heart to be pumped to the rest of the body. Blood pressure can be measured by measuring systolic over diastolic pressure. Systolic pressure is the pressure in the blood vessels while the hearts beats while diastolic pressure is measured by the pressure after the heart beat. Diastolic pressure is also called the “filling stage” because it occurs when the heart is in a state of relaxation and the semi-lunar valve is closed. In opposition, systolic pressure transpires during contraction where the tricuspid and bicuspid valves are closed.
Heart Rate, read by taking a pulse either on neck, arm or wrist (but most precise measurement is taken from arm), uses arterial pressure to calculate the rate that blood is flowing through the arteries. It differs from blood pressure and is the number of times the heart beats in one minute.
This muscle is pertinent to survival and therefore been tested and experimented on habitually. Many tests have been done on the heart, including but not limited to the effect of respiratory ailments and stress on heart rate (Weber 1985). The results show, conclusively, that heart rate and blood pressure are directly correlated with stress, meaning that when someone is stressed the harder their heart pumps and the more pressure they are putting on it. Nevertheless, there are many other factors that affect heart rate and blood pressure. Most of the factors discussed share a direct correlation with heart rate (HR or referred to sometimes as pulse rate PR) and blood pressure (BP).
Get Help With Your Essay
If you need assistance with writing your essay, our professional essay writing service is here to help!
There are also some lifetime diseases that stem from a root of vascular problems that were before thought to have little to no affect (Skoog 1996). For instance, a study conducted about Dementia revealed that a higher BP affected and was in correlation with the development of Dementia. There is more evidence that supports this claim, but it has yet to be proven. Despite the ambiguity of the correlation of BP and Dementia, there is many health related concerns that surrounds BP and HR.
As the research of BP and HR becomes more involved, so do the experiments concerning them. In the experiment conducted in LIFE103, a biology class at Colorado State University, 27 subjects tested the affects of hydration on BP and HR/PR. It is hypothesized that there will be a significant change in both BP and HR and predicted that BP and HR will decrease after the addition of water into the body.
Materials and Methods
All variables were kept as similar as the environment would allow and subjects didn’t consumer water for two hours before experimentation was started. There was an uneven mixture of male and female and all students were of different weights, but roughly fell into a broad spectrum of “normal” weight. Three basal readings were taken from the 27 participants. The participants measured themselves using a sphygmomanometer and recorded both heart rate and blood pressure. These were then averaged and if the average was off, additional measurements were taken.
Heart rate and blood pressure served as the independent variables, while the dependent variables were the water and the environmental factors.
After basal measurements were completed students drank 2 cups of water. Water temperatures varied from room temperature to cold and data was separated according what temperature water subject drank. The subjects were given five minutes to consume the water.
A reading was taken directly after the water was completely consumed. Readings of BP and PR were taken in increments of three minutes since the initial reading until the time struck twelve minutes. There were five total readings. Information from all of the subjects was gathered and charted.
Data was analyzed and T tested. Probabilities were found and then information was compiled into a list of results. Excel was used to assemble the results and create graphs and tables.
Results
The experiment took approximately twelve minutes after water was consumed. The 27 subjects did not drink anything for at least two hours prior, dehydration levels, however were minimal. Hydration levels after drinking approximately two ounces of water were also minimal. Differences between heart rate and pulse rate are not obvious between dehydration and hydration.
The pulse rate (or heart rate) had a more visible change. Comparing averages of final PR (twelve minutes after water was drunk) to the basal reading yielded a change, but it was within a hundredth of a decimal point. The change in blood pressure was also nonexistent. Though, if compared the values of diastolic and systolic pressure change more significantly than the values of the PR.
The data is summarized below in a mixture of charts and written statistics.
This simply surmises that although the average of all of the basal readings are less than the average of the completed experiment pulse rate, they are inconsistent and very skewed (being that the middle value is much larger than the other two, making the data have outliers.
The chart below shows the change of pulse rate after the water was consumed. There is no significant change as the time progresses.
Average of PR taken at times 0,3,6,9 and 12 were used compared to the time that the experiment progressed over.
Comparatively, blood pressure had similar results. On a time scale, blood pressure had no distinctive change. The chart on top is the change in systolic pressure over time while the chart on the bottom is the change of diastolic pressure over the time stretch that the experiment was done in. It shows that nothing can be conclusively found about either pressure because of their roller coaster motion.
Discussion
As earlier hypothesized blood pressure will change and so will heart rate, however they won’t be affected heavily because of the small amount of change that is being inflicted upon the subjects. It was predicted that after an amount of time without drinking any water the subject’s heart rate will speed and after water has been added, heart rate will slow down.
The results were inconclusive, the changes were so minimal that (when averaged) the difference between the beginning pulse rate and the final pulse rate were within hundredths of a decimal of each other. Blood pressure had little change. Systolic pressure decreased slightly as did Diastolic, but it was too such a degree that the change could be accounted by many variables. Drawing these conclusions, earlier hypothesis and prediction cannot be supported.
Doing a T test, the information is shown to be probable (P <.05), however the more T tests performed comparing the beginning data with the end result (going from left to right on the table), the T tests score above the optimal range.
PR T Test
PR vs PR
PR1 Basal vs PR 12 min
PR2 Basal vs PR12 min
PR3 Basal vs PR12 min
PR0 min vs PR12 min
PR3 min vs PR12 min
PR6 min vs PR12 min
T Test
.00028
.00028
.00079
.772211
.979664
.413052
T test scores based on excel calculations concerning an experiment performed in class. (P<.05) makes the probability of legitimacy high.
Alternatively, the experiment could have been bettered with more control. There were many variables that were not accounted for. Water temperature, amount of water drunk, amount of exercise allowed to be exerted by the subjects, sex of subjects, size of subjects and normal heart rate and blood pressure of subjects were not consolidated in the experiment. This all accounts for experimental error, for the hypothesis and predictions are correct according to many experiments done by professional, scientists. These experiments have slightly different variables and test for slightly different results, but there results are conclusive to what should have been found in the experiment conducted in class.
Find Out How UKEssays.com Can Help You!
Our academic experts are ready and waiting to assist with any writing project you may have. From simple essay plans, through to full dissertations, you can guarantee we have a service perfectly matched to your needs.
View our academic writing services
In opposition to hydration, dehydration has a lasting effect on heart rate as well (Montain 1992). Understanding how dehydration affects heart rate is pertinent to understanding how hydration affects it. Expectations would be that they are complementary of each other and this study titled “Influence of graded dehydration on hyperthermia and cardiovascular drift during excursive” focuses on the dehydration aspect on the body and how the heart rate and blood pressure react. Hyperthermia is another aspect of this experiment, but seeing as it doesn’t pertain to the experiment conducted in class, it will be overlooked.
Directly pulling information from the Montian experiment, there was a certain number of cyclists that took part in the experiment, but they all fell into the same category: capable, normally low heart rate people (Montain 1992). Though there was no difference in the amount of sweat that the cyclists produced, there was a significant difference in their heart rates based on what amount of liquid that they consumed (there was four different groups, each assigned to a different amount of water). The study shows that the level of dehydration directly correlates with Heart Rate. The amount of water in the blood indirectly correlates with Heart Rate meaning that the less water there is the harder the heart has to beat in order to get the stagnant blood through the veins and arteries and into the entire body.
The x-axis is time (out of 110), this shows that heart rate. The heart rate increases with the smaller amount of fluid.
An experiment performed by Ethan R. Nadel, Suzanne M. Fortney and C. Bruce Wenger similarly tests the effects of hyperhydration and hypohydration on the heart rate and the circulatory system, however, the investigation compares and contrasts the two different conditions other than going into depth in one over the other (Nadel 1980). Maximal arm blood flow was taken for both of the experiments and when in a hypohydration condition, the blood flow was reduced by almost 50%. Though there was a difference in the amount of blood when hyperhydration conditions were met (subject drank 2 liters of water), the difference was not significant enough to make a large impact on the subject’s heart rate. However, when the subject then exercises the heart rate is abnormally slow compared to normal and hypohydration conditions. A sufficient amount of water also helps maintain a constant internal temperature. Temperature is actually a very good way to measure the blood pressure and as seen in the chart below blood flow can be measured against temperature to find that the less water a person has, the higher the temperature is.
Measures blood flow versus temperature (rectum or esophageal temperature is always used in cases like this).
An earlier experiment performed in the early 1940’s noted in “Effect of hydration state on circulatory and thermal regulations” studied the same topic, but in a different manor (Nadel 1980). Their findings were conclusive that even when hypohydration conditions are achieved, when water is given to the subject in large quantities, hyperhydration conditions are easily established and a quick rebound of water replacement in the body takes place. In fact, the body reaches a steady condition in much less time than it takes the body to get into a critical condition. This illustrates the fast effects of water retake in the heart rate and body.
These detailed experiments are crucial in helping to understand what went wrong in the classroom experiment. The study size, though it certainly inflicted the value of the data, was not the problem, but the quality of the subjects. They were all of different athletic potential and did not have similar heart rates or blood pressure rates. The collection procedure itself could also have some error. Subjects measured their own pressures and rates and did not exactly time it between each three minute intervals. There was also not enough time to collect sufficient data (hours of dedication were needed, not minuet minutes).
Data and conclusions drawn from this experiment are skewed and cannot accurately portray what happens when a sudden increase in hydration happens in the body. Therefore the null hypothesis has been failed to be rejected.
Sources Cited
- Weber KT, Janicki JS. 1985. Cardiopulmonary exercise testing for evaluation of chronic cardiac failure. The American journal of cardiology 55: Issue 2.
- Weedman Donna, Sokoloski Erica Smith. 2009. Biology of Organisms: A Laboratory Manual for LIFE103. Mason, OH: Cengage Learning. p 173 – 184.
- Skoog MD, L. Nilsson MD, G. Persson MD, B. Lernfelt MD, S. Landahl MD, B. Palmertz MD, L-A. Andreasson MD, A. Odén PhD and A. Svanborg MD. 1996. 15-year longitudinal study of blood pressure and dementia. The Lancet 347: 1141 – 1145.
- Montian J Scott and Coyle F Edward. 1992. Influence of graded dehydration on hyperthermia and cardiovascular drift during exercise. Journal of Applied Physiology 73: Issue 4.
- Nadel E R, Fortney M S, Wenger C B. 1980. Effect of hydration state of circulatory and thermal regulations. Journal of Applied Physiology 49: Issue 4.
Cite This Work
To export a reference to this article please select a referencing style below: